Thursday, September 25, 2014

Social vs. Natural sciences

Laura Benedict
IDS 802
Dr. P. Tramel
Homework 2
The Superiority of Social Science
Humans are inherently interested in scientific principles. Every child begins asking the all-important question “why” multiple times a day, sometimes for the same concept repeatedly. On this note, Carl Sagan stated, “Every kid starts out as a natural born-scientist, and then we beat it out of them. A few trickle through the system with their wonder and enthusiasm for science intact,” (Psychology Today, 1996, p. 33). The mind boggles at the breadth and depth of scientific questions. It might be primarily for this reason that most of us allow other commitments and challenges to entice us away from science as a vocation.
If you think of “Science” as an Earth, you can then think of the major disciplines such as physics, geology, and biology as continents. The one thread that holds the entire planet together, the solid metal inner core of the Earth, is the scientific method. Mathematics and English do not belong on the same planet, they do not even belong in the same orbit. They are subjects as different in kind as to be almost completely oblivious of the existence of the other. This is not so with science. No matter how different astrophysics is from abnormal psychology, they belong on the same planet. Just as geological continents are segmented into countries, cities, and then neighborhoods, scientific disciplines are similarly segmented into ever more specialized fields. This essay will effectively compare the East and the West hemispheres. As a graduate student, I know that last statement is an Americentric misnomer: East and West are relative terms. The sun shines equally on the whole Earth around the equatorial region. Similarly, each scientific field of inquiry is equally important.
Even though scientific wonder is inherent, scientific thinking and knowledge is not. “Science is a way of thinking much more than it is a body of knowledge,” (Sagan, 1994, p. 104). Scientific thinking requires going beyond the original statement of fact, it requires digging deeper into the subject, as well as around the subject. The scientific thinker will ask if the statement is really true, if there are other possible explanations for the same conclusion, and how would the conclusion differ if one small detail is changed. In short, scientific thinking requires thinking critically. This level of thinking, questioning, and researching does not tend to make friends among peers or instructors, especially at the grade-school level. For this reason, many interested in scientific endeavors are dissuaded by peer pressure.
Another form of dissuasion from the study of many of the natural sciences is the language used. Digging too deeply into the natural sciences will bring the student face-to-face with large, difficult to read words, which require other large, difficult to read words in order to define and understand (Machlup). Meanwhile, social sciences provide a friendlier window into the world of science because the jargon is frequently used in the news, on social media, and in every day conversation. These concepts still require deeper learning to fully understand, but the layperson feels he or she can grasp the basics earlier, providing much needed confidence (Machlup). Students of natural sciences frequently disdain the social sciences because the social sciences display greater variability in their observed data, they do not formulate general theories within their fields, and they have no constant numerical relationships on which to rely. However, despite this apparent disdain, we will see that the social sciences hold a valid place in the scientific world, arguably a loftier place. The social sciences use the same basic principles to search for answers to different questions than the natural sciences, but they add more to every answer than the natural sciences can ever achieve.

Variability of Observed Data
The basic method of scientific inquiry is generally and broadly described as the scientific method. This is a general outline of a course of action in answering any question as empirically as is possible at the current time. In my High School basic sciences class, my teacher amazed us all by creating a vacuum, and pulling a boiled egg through the narrow neck of a bottle merely by the use of a match. I made peanut brittle and ice cream in chemistry class by closely following the instructions written in chemical notation, instead of kitchen recipe format. These classroom “experiments” are easily replicated to find the exact same results. Not all scientific research is as easily replicated. At this moment, I can pluck from my archives scholarly sources on such subjects as the efficacy of teaching ethical decision making skills at the post-secondary level, the benefits of utilizing authoritarian parenting styles versus authoritative or permissive parenting styles, and a brief overview of string theory. None of these research projects are easily replicated; not all of them are classified as social science.
Machlup views the social sciences to be inferior to the natural sciences because while conducting experiments, social scientists find it impossible to control for every relevant variable. Scriven agrees with Machlup in that the considerably greater number of relevant variables inherent in the social sciences makes primary experimentation, therefore experiment replication, nearly impossible. However, he feels that these extra variables add a richness and dimension, found only rarely in the natural sciences. The conclusions found after analyzing field observations are richer, deeper, and more meaningful than the clinical reporting of data and facts found from the natural sciences. Indeed, where the natural sciences fill you with awe and wonder, and entice you with marvelous facts, the social sciences do so over a cozy warm beverage of choice, and the comforting company of friends.

Theory Formulation
The natural sciences are replete with general theories to explain how the world works, but the social sciences are more apt to ask the question, “why”. This is the very question that will strike fear and dread in the heart of a weary parent who does not know the answer, and does not care. This is the same question that keeps social scientists up for days on end, seemingly tirelessly, searching for a glimmer of an answer. The short answer is, there is no short answer. It is for this reason that many natural scientists look down their noses at the social scientists who do not care, because they are too busy searching for answers with a gleam in their eyes. Fay and Moon write that the natural sciences formulate a theory by considering theory and measurement. However, they add that the social sciences must also consider the identity and meaning of the actions so closely observed by the researcher. These actions must be identified and defined by the social actors themselves, or the context is lost and the observation is useless. On this note, Machlup seems truly saddened that this dimension is non-quantifiable. As an economist, he is used to dealing almost exclusively with numbers and their meanings, thus aesthetic and poignant depths are likely impenetrable to him.

Constant Numerical Relationships
This subject seems to be a favorite of Machlup, given his chosen field. He writes of the velocity of light, Planck’s constant, the gravitational constant (which I argue is only constant on this planet), the Coulomb constant, proton mass, the ratio of proton mass to electron mass, and the fine-structure constant. “Sociologists…have reported some ‘relatively stable’ ratios…but the ‘stability’ is only relative to the extreme variability of other numerical ratios,” (Machlup, p. 13).
This suggests that the lack of numerical constants implies a social science inferiority to natural sciences. However, numerical constants are not the only constants that matter. Machlup does not overtly provide a reason for requiring numerical constants, but he implies that they simplify mathematical computations. However, mathematical constructs are only one small part of the whole picture. Constants other than numbers are called archetypes; for instance, behavior archetypes such as the Mother and the Warrior. Personality archetypes are also familiar to many people by their letter notations such as ENTJ and ISFP. Additionally, social groups always separate into a hierarchy in consistent ways, such as by monetary worth, by brute strength, or by something more arbitrary such as color of skin. The constants found in social sciences are more interesting, provide more information, and are more usable than are the constants found in mathematics.

Conclusion
Our scientific Earth is held together by the basic principle of gathering empirical evidence: the scientific method. The higher variability of observed data in the social sciences leads to richer, deeper, more meaningful conclusions than are found in the natural sciences. The general theories found in natural sciences answer “how”, but social sciences are more interested in answering “why”. The social sciences have more interesting, more informative, and more usable constants than do the natural sciences. Therefore, the social sciences use the same basic principles to search for answers to different questions than the natural sciences, but they add more to every answer than the natural sciences can ever achieve.



References
Fay, B., & Moon, D. J. (1994). What would an adequate philosophy of social science look like? In M. Martin & L. C. McIntyre (Eds.), Readings in the philosophy of social science (pp. 21-35). Massachusetts Institute of Technology: Bradford.
Machlup, F. (1994). Are the social sciences really inferior? In M. Martin & L. C. McIntyre (Eds.), Readings in the philosophy of social science (pp. 5-19). Massachusetts Institute of Technology: Bradford.
Psychology Today, (1996, Jan-Feb). A slayer of demons. Psychology Today, 29(1), 30-66.  
Sagan, C. (1994). Can we know the universe? Reflections on a grain of salt. In M. Gardner (Ed.) Great essays in science (pp. 102-109). Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.

Scriven, M. (1994). A possible distinction between traditional scientific disciplines and the study of human behavior. In M. Martin & L. C. McIntyre (Eds.), Readings in the philosophy of social science (pp. 71-77). Massachusetts Institute of Technology: Bradford